Philosophy:Biology and sexual orientation
Template:Sexual orientationTemplate:LGBT sidebar The relationship between biology and sexual orientation is a subject of on-going research. While scientists do not know the exact cause of sexual orientation, they theorize that it is caused by a complex interplay of genetic, hormonal, and environmental influences.[1][2][3] However, evidence is weak for hypotheses that the post-natal social environment impacts sexual orientation, especially for males.[4]
Biological theories for explaining the causes of sexual orientation are favored by scientists.[1] These factors, which may be related to the development of a sexual orientation, include genes, the early uterine environment (such as prenatal hormones), and brain structure.
Scientific research and studies
Fetal development and hormones
The influence of hormones on the developing fetus has been the most influential causal hypothesis of the development of sexual orientation.[4][5] In simple terms, the developing fetal brain begins in a "female" state. Both INAH3 (third interstitial nucleus of the anterior hypothalamus) area on the left side of the hypothalamus, which stores gender preference, and the center area of the bed stria terminalus (BSTc) area on the right side of the hypothalamus, which stores gender identity, are undeveloped and function as female. The action of the SRY gene in the Y-chromosome in the fetus prompts the development of testes, which release testosterone, the primary androgen receptor-activating hormone, to allow testosterone to enter the cells and masculinize the fetus and fetal brain. If a sufficient amount of testosterone is received by INAH3 at 12 weeks following conception, the testosterone stimulates the enlargement of INAH3, which is known to be involved in directing typical male sex behavior, such as attraction to females. If INAH3 does not receive enough testosterone to override the circulating estrogen, it may not grow to the size typically observed in males. Subsequently, INAH3 may function as female or partially female, potentially causing same-sex attraction to males. Although, the size of INAH3 in homosexual men compared to heterosexual men may not be statistically different, though homosexual men may have a greater cell density, that is, more cells per unit volume, but a similar total number of neurons in INAH3 than heterosexual men.[6]
Studies have shown that INAH3 in gay men has likely been exposed to low levels of testosterone in the brain compared to straight men, or had different levels of receptivity to its masculinizing effects, or experienced hormone fluctuations at critical times during fetal development. In women, if INAH3 receives more testosterone than is normal for females, INAH3 may enlarge somewhat or even to the size that is normal for males, increasing the likelihood of same sex attraction.[4] Supporting this are studies of the finger digit ratio of the right hand, which is a robust marker of prenatal testosterone exposure [disputed ]. Lesbians on average, have significantly more masculine digit ratios, a finding which has been replicated numerous times in studies cross-culturally.[7] While direct effects are hard to measure for ethical reasons, animal experiments where scientists manipulate exposure to sex hormones during gestation can also induce lifelong male-typical behavior and mounting in female animals, and female-typical behavior in male animals.[4][7][5][8]
Maternal immune responses during fetal development are strongly demonstrated as causing male homosexuality and bisexuality.[9] Research since the 1990s has demonstrated that the more sons a woman has, there is a higher chance of later born sons being gay. During pregnancy, male cells enter a mother's bloodstream, which are foreign to her immune system. In response, she develops antibodies to neutralize them. These antibodies are then released on future male fetuses and may neutralize Y-linked antigens, which play a role in brain masculinization, leaving areas of the brain responsible for sexual attraction in the female-typical position, i.e., expressing attraction to men. The more sons a mother has will increase the levels of these antibodies, thus creating the observed fraternal birth order effect. Biochemical evidence to support this effect was confirmed in a lab study in 2017, finding that mothers with a gay son, particularly those with older brothers, had heightened levels of antibodies to the NLGN4Y Y-protein than mothers with heterosexual sons.[9][10] J. Michael Bailey has described maternal immune responses as "causal" of male homosexuality.[11] This effect is estimated to account for between 15 and 29% of gay men, while other gay and bisexual men are thought to owe sexual orientation to genetic and hormonal interactions.[12][9]
Socialization theories, which were dominant in the 1900s, favored the idea that children were born "undifferentiated" and were socialized into gender roles and sexual orientation. This led to medical experiments in which newborn and infant boys were surgically reassigned into girls after accidents such as botched circumcisions. These males were then reared and raised as females without telling the boys, which, contrary to expectations, did not make them feminine nor attracted to men. All published cases providing sexual orientation grew up to be strongly attracted to women. The failure of these experiments demonstrate that socialization effects do not induce feminine type behavior in males, nor make them attracted to men, and that the organizational effects of hormones on the fetal brain prior to birth have permanent effects. These are indicative of 'nature', not nurture, at least with regards to male sexual orientation.[4]
The sexually dimorphic nucleus of the preoptic area (SDN-POA) is a key region of the brain which differs between males and females in humans and a number of mammals (e.g., sheep/rams, mice, rats), and is caused by sex differences in hormone exposure.[4][7] The INAH-3 region is bigger in males than in females, and is known to be a critical region in sexual behavior. Dissection studies found that gay men had significantly smaller sized INAH-3 than heterosexual males, which is shifted in the female typical direction, a finding first demonstrated by neuroscientist Simon LeVay, which has been replicated.[7] Dissection studies are rare, however, due to lack of funding and brain samples.[4]
Long-term studies of homosexual behavior in domesticated sheep led by Charles Roselli have found that 6-8% of rams have a homosexual preference through their life. Dissection of ram brains also found a similar smaller (feminized) structure in homosexually oriented rams compared to heterosexually oriented rams in the equivalent brain region to the human SDN, the ovine sexually dimorphic nucleus (oSDN).[13]:107–110 The size of the sheep oSDN has also been demonstrated to be formed in utero, rather than postnatally, underscoring the role of prenatal hormones in masculinization of the brain for sexual attraction.[8][4]
Other studies in humans have relied on brain imaging technology, such as research led by Ivanka Savic which compared hemispheres of the brain. This research found that straight men had right hemispheres 2% larger than the left, described as modest but "highly significant difference" by LeVay. In heterosexual women, the two hemispheres were the same size. In gay men, the two hemispheres were also the same size, or sex atypical, while in lesbians, the right hemispheres were slightly larger than the left, indicating a small shift in the male direction.[13]:112
A model proposed by evolutionary geneticist William R. Rice argues that a misexpressed epigenetic modifier of testosterone sensitivity or insensitivity that affected development of the brain can explain homosexuality, and can best explain twin discordance.[15] Rice et al. propose that these epimarks normally canalize sexual development, preventing intersex conditions in most of the population, but sometimes failing to erase across generations and causing reversed sexual preference.[15] On grounds of evolutionary plausibility, Gavrilets, Friberg and Rice argue that all mechanisms for exclusive homosexual orientations likely trace back to their epigenetic model.[16] Testing this hypothesis is possible with current stem cell technology.[17]
Prenatal thyroid theory
Prenatal thyroid theory of same-sex attraction/gender dysphoria has been based on clinical and developmental observations of youngsters presenting to child psychiatry clinics in Istanbul/Turkey. The report of 12 cases with same-sex attraction/gender dysphoria born to mothers with thyroid diseases was first presented in EPA Congress, Vienna (2015) and published as an article in the same year.[18][19] The extremely significant relationship between the two conditions suggested an independent model, named as Prenatal Thyroid Model of Homosexuality. According to Turkish child & adolescent psychiatrist Osman Sabuncuoglu, who generated the theory, maternal thyroid dysfunction may lead to abnormal deviations from gender-specific development in the offspring. Autoimmune destructive process as seen in Hashimoto thyroiditis, diminished supply of thyroid hormones and impacts on prenatal androgen system were all considered as contributing mechanisms. In a follow-up theoretical paper,[20] previous research findings indicating higher rates of polycystic ovary syndrome (PCOS) in female-to-male transsexuals and lesbian women were conceived as an indication of Prenatal Thyroid Model since PCOS and autoimmune thyroiditis are frequently comorbid diseases. Likewise, increased rates of autism spectrum disorder in children born to mothers with thyroid dysfunction and overrepresentation of ASD individuals in gender dysphoria populations suggest such an association. A second group of young children with this pattern were presented in IACAPAP Congress,[21] Prague (2018).
The findings from previous research in LGBT populations had called for attention to be paid to thyroid system.[22][23] A commentary by Jeffrey Mullen, published shortly after the 2015 article, underlined the importance of Prenatal Thyroid Model and supported developments in this field.[24] Afterwards, several authors have emphasized the role of thyroid system in sexuality while citing the Prenatal Thyroid Model.[25][26][27][28] Among them, Carosa et al. concluded that thyroid hormones, affecting the human sexual function strongly, the thyroid gland must be considered, along with the genitals and the brain, a sexual organ.[25] As a tertiary source, an authoritative book on the subject of interplay between endocrinology, brain and behavior has also cited the thyroid-homosexuality proposal article in the latest edition.[29] Most importantly, a genome-wide genetic association study on male homosexuals identified a significant region on Chromosome 14 which is related to autoimmune thyroid dysfunction in human beings.[30] This is apparently a big support to the Prenatal Thyroid Model.
Genetic influences
Multiple genes have been found to play a role in sexual orientation. Scientists caution that many people misconstrue the meanings of genetic and environmental.[4] Environmental influence does not automatically imply that the social environment influences or contributes to the development of sexual orientation. Hypotheses for the impact of the post-natal social environment on sexual orientation are weak, especially for males.[4] There is, however, a vast non-social environment that is non-genetic yet still biological, such as prenatal development, that likely helps shape sexual orientation.[4]:76
Twin studies
A number of twin studies have attempted to compare the relative importance of genetics and environment in the determination of sexual orientation. In a 1991 study, Bailey and Pillard conducted a study of male twins recruited from "homophile publications", and found that 52% of monozygotic (MZ) brothers (of whom 59 were questioned) and 22% of the dizygotic (DZ) twins were concordant for homosexuality.[31] 'MZ' indicates identical twins with the same sets of genes and 'DZ' indicates fraternal twins where genes are mixed to an extent similar to that of non-twin siblings. In a study of 61 pairs of twins, researchers found among their mostly male subjects a concordance rate for homosexuality of 66% among monozygotic twins and a 30% one among dizygotic twins.[32] In 2000, Bailey, Dunne and Martin studied a larger sample of 4,901 Australian twins but reported less than half the level of concordance.[33] They found 20% concordance in the male identical or MZ twins and 24% concordance for the female identical or MZ twins. Self reported zygosity, sexual attraction, fantasy and behaviours were assessed by questionnaire and zygosity was serologically checked when in doubt. Other researchers support biological causes for both men and women's sexual orientation.[34]
A 2008 study of all adult twins in Sweden (more than 7,600 twins)[35] found that same-sex behaviour was explained by both heritable genetic factors and unique environmental factors (which can include the prenatal environment during gestation, exposure to illness in early life, peer groups not shared with a twin, etc.), although a twin study cannot identify which factor is at play. Influences of the shared environment (influences including the family environment, rearing, shared peer groups, culture and societal views, and sharing the same school and community) had no effect for men, and a weak effect for women. This is consistent with the common finding that parenting and culture appears to play no role in male sexual orientation, but may play some small role in women. The study concludes that genetic influences on any lifetime same-sex partner were stronger for men than women, and that "it has been suggested individual differences in heterosexual and homosexual behavior result from unique environmental factors such as prenatal exposure to sex hormones, progressive maternal immunization to sex-specific proteins, or neurodevelopmental factors", although does not rule out other variables. The use of all adult twins in Sweden was designed to address the criticism of volunteer studies, in which a potential bias towards participation by gay twins may influence the results:
Biometric modeling revealed that, in men, genetic effects explained .34–.39 of the variance [of sexual orientation], the shared environment .00, and the individual-specific environment .61–.66 of the variance. Corresponding estimates among women were .18–.19 for genetic factors, .16–.17 for shared environmental, and .64–.66 for unique environmental factors. Although wide confidence intervals suggest cautious interpretation, the results are consistent with moderate, primarily genetic, familial effects, and moderate to large effects of the nonshared environment (social and biological) on same-sex sexual behavior.[35]
Chromosome linkage studies
Chromosome linkage studies of sexual orientation have indicated the presence of multiple contributing genetic factors throughout the genome. In 1993, Dean Hamer and colleagues published findings from a linkage analysis of a sample of 76 gay brothers and their families.[36] Hamer et al. found that the gay men had more gay male uncles and cousins on the maternal side of the family than on the paternal side. Gay brothers who showed this maternal pedigree were then tested for X chromosome linkage, using twenty-two markers on the X chromosome to test for similar alleles. In another finding, thirty-three of the forty sibling pairs tested were found to have similar alleles in the distal region of Xq28, which was significantly higher than the expected rates of 50% for fraternal brothers. This was popularly dubbed the "gay gene" in the media, causing significant controversy. In 1998, Sanders et al.[citation needed] reported on their similar study, in which they found that 13% of uncles of gay brothers on the maternal side were homosexual, compared with 6% on the paternal side.[37]
A later analysis by Hu et al. replicated and refined the earlier findings. This study revealed that 67% of gay brothers in a new saturated sample shared a marker on the X chromosome at Xq28.[38] Two other studies (Bailey et al., 1999; McKnight and Malcolm, 2000) failed to find a preponderance of gay relatives in the maternal line of homosexual men.[37] One study by Rice et al. in 1999 failed to replicate the Xq28 linkage results.[39] Meta-analysis of all available linkage data indicates a significant link to Xq28, but also indicates that additional genes must be present to account for the full heritability of sexual orientation.[40]
Mustanski et al. (2005) performed a full-genome scan (instead of just an X chromosome scan) on individuals and families previously reported on in Hamer et al. (1993) and Hu et al. (1995), as well as additional new subjects. In the full sample they did not find linkage to Xq28.[41]
Results from the first large, comprehensive multi-center genetic linkage study of male sexual orientation were reported by an independent group of researchers at the American Society of Human Genetics in 2012.[42] The study population included 409 independent pairs of gay brothers, who were analyzed with over 300,000 single-nucleotide polymorphism markers. The data strongly replicated Hamer's Xq28 findings as determined by both two-point and multipoint (MERLIN) LOD score mapping. Significant linkage was also detected in the pericentromeric region of chromosome 8, overlapping with one of the regions detected in the Hamer lab's previous genomewide study. The authors concluded that "our findings, taken in context with previous work, suggest that genetic variation in each of these regions contributes to development of the important psychological trait of male sexual orientation". Female sexual orientation does not seem to be linked to Xq28,[38][43] though it does appear moderately heritable.[42]
In addition to sex chromosomal contribution, a potential autosomal genetic contribution to the development of homosexual orientation has also been suggested. In a study population composed of more than 7000 participants, Ellis et al. (2008) found a statistically significant difference in the frequency of blood type A between homosexuals and heterosexuals. They also found that "unusually high" proportions of homosexual males and homosexual females were Rh negative in comparison to heterosexuals. As both blood type and Rh factor are genetically inherited traits controlled by alleles located on chromosome 9 and chromosome 1 respectively, the study indicates a potential link between genes on autosomes and homosexuality.[44][45]
The biology of sexual orientation has been studied in detail in several animal model systems. In the common fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster, the complete pathway of sexual differentiation of the brain and the behaviors it controls is well established in both males and females, providing a concise model of biologically controlled courtship.[46] In mammals, a group of geneticists at the Korea Advanced Institute of Science and Technology bred a female mice specifically lacking a particular gene related to sexual behavior. Without the gene, the mice exhibited masculine sexual behavior and attraction toward urine of other female mice. Those mice who retained the gene fucose mutarotase (FucM) were attracted to male mice.[47]
In interviews to the press, researchers have pointed that the evidence of genetic influences should not be equated with genetic determinism. According to Dean Hamer and Michael Bailey, genetic aspects are only one of the multiple causes of homosexuality.[48][49]
In 2017, Scientific Reports published an article with a genome wide association study on male sexual orientation. The research consisted of 1,077 homosexual men and 1,231 heterosexual men. A gene named SLITRK6 on chromosome 13 was identified.[30] The research supports another study which had been done by the neuroscientist Simon LeVay. LeVay's research suggested that the hypothalamus of gay men is different from straight men.[50] The SLITRK6 is active in the mid-brain where the hypothalamus is. The researchers found that the thyroid stimulating hormone receptor (TSHR) on chromosome 14 shows sequence differences between gay and straight men.[30] Graves' disease is associated with TSHR abnormalities, with previous research indicating that Graves' disease is more common in gay men than in straight men.[51] Research indicated that gay people have lower body weight than straight people. It had been suggested that the overactive TSHR hormone lowered body weight in gay people, though this remains unproven.[52][53]
In 2018, Ganna et al. performed another genome-wide association study on sexual orientation of men and women with data from 26,890 people who had at least one same-sex partner and 450,939 controls. The data in the study was meta-analyzed and obtained from the UK Biobank study and 23andMe. The researchers identified four variants more common in people who reported at least one same-sex experience on chromosomes 7, 11, 12, and 15. The variants on chromosomes 11 and 15 were specific to men, with the variant on chromosome 11 located in an olfactory gene and the variant on chromosome 15 having previously been linked to male-pattern baldness. The four variants were also correlated with mood and mental health disorders; major depressive disorder and schizophrenia in men and women, and bipolar disorder in women. However, none of the four variants could reliably predict sexual orientation.[54]
In August 2019, a genome-wide association study of 493,001 individuals concluded that hundreds or thousands of genetic variants underlie homosexual behavior in both sexes, with 5 variants in particular being significantly associated. Some of these variants had sex-specific effects, and two of these variants suggested links to biological pathways that involve sex hormone regulation and olfaction. All the variants together captured between 8 and 25% of the variation in individual differences in homosexual behavior. These genes partly overlap with those for several other traits, including openness to experience and risk-taking behavior. Additional analyses suggested that sexual behavior, attraction, identity, and fantasies are influenced by a similar set of genetic variants. They also found that the genetic effects that differentiate heterosexual from homosexual behavior are not the same as those that differ among nonheterosexuals with lower versus higher proportions of same-sex partners, which suggests that there is no single continuum from heterosexual to homosexual preference, as suggested by the Kinsey scale.[55]
In October 2021, another research paper reported that genetic factors influence the development of same-sex sexual behavior. A two-stage genome-wide association study (GWAS) with a total sample of 1478 homosexual males and 3313 heterosexual males in Han Chinese populations identified two genetic loci (FMR1NB and ZNF536) showing consistent association with male sexual orientation.[56]
Epigenetics studies
A study suggests linkage between a mother's genetic make-up and homosexuality of her sons. Women have two X chromosomes, one of which is "switched off". The inactivation of the X chromosome occurs randomly throughout the embryo, resulting in cells that are mosaic with respect to which chromosome is active. In some cases though, it appears that this switching off can occur in a non-random fashion. Bocklandt et al. (2006) reported that, in mothers of homosexual men, the number of women with extreme skewing of X chromosome inactivation is significantly higher than in mothers without gay sons. In a study of 94 participants, 13% of mothers with one gay son, and 23% of mothers with two gay sons, showed extreme skewing, compared to 4% of mothers without gay sons.[57]
Birth order
Blanchard and Klassen (1997) reported that each additional older brother increases the odds of a man being gay by 33%.[58][59] This is now "one of the most reliable epidemiological variables ever identified in the study of sexual orientation".[60] To explain this finding, it has been proposed that male fetuses provoke a maternal immune reaction that becomes stronger with each successive male fetus. This maternal immunization hypothesis (MIH) begins when cells from a male fetus enter the mother's circulation during pregnancy or while giving birth.[61] Male fetuses produce H-Y antigens which are "almost certainly involved in the sexual differentiation of vertebrates". These Y-linked proteins would not be recognized in the mother's immune system because she is female, causing her to develop antibodies which would travel through the placental barrier into the fetal compartment. From here, the anti-male bodies would then cross the blood/brain barrier (BBB) of the developing fetal brain, altering sex-dimorphic brain structures relative to sexual orientation, increasing the likelihood that the exposed son will be more attracted to men than women.[61] It is this antigen which maternal H-Y antibodies are proposed to both react to and 'remember'. Successive male fetuses are then attacked by H-Y antibodies which somehow decrease the ability of H-Y antigens to perform their usual function in brain masculinization.[58]
In 2017, researchers discovered a biological mechanism of gay people who tend to have older brothers. They think Neuroligin 4 Y-linked protein is responsible for a later son being gay. They found that women had significantly higher anti-NLGN4Y levels than men. In addition, mothers of gay sons, particularly those with older brothers, had significantly higher anti-NLGN4Y levels than did the control samples of women, including mothers of heterosexual sons. The results suggest an association between a maternal immune response to NLGN4Y and subsequent sexual orientation in male offspring.[10]
The fraternal birth order effect, however, does not apply to instances where a firstborn is homosexual.[62][63][64][65]
Female fertility
In 2004, Italian researchers conducted a study of about 4,600 people who were the relatives of 98 homosexual and 100 heterosexual men. Female relatives of the homosexual men tended to have more offspring than those of the heterosexual men. Female relatives of the homosexual men on their mother's side tended to have more offspring than those on the father's side. The researchers concluded that there was genetic material being passed down on the X chromosome which both promote fertility in the mother and homosexuality in her male offspring. The connections discovered would explain about 20% of the cases studied, indicating that it being a highly significant factor to account for, but not the sole genetic factor determining sexual orientation.[66][67]
Pheromone studies
Research conducted in Sweden[68] has suggested that gay and straight men respond differently to two odors that are believed to be involved in sexual arousal. The research showed that when both heterosexual women and gay men are exposed to a testosterone derivative found in men's sweat, a region in the hypothalamus is activated. Heterosexual men, on the other hand, have a similar response to an estrogen-like compound found in women's urine.[69] The conclusion is that sexual attraction, whether same-sex or opposite-sex oriented, operates similarly on a biological level. Researchers have suggested that this possibility could be further explored by studying young subjects to see if similar responses in the hypothalamus are found and then correlating these data with adult sexual orientation.[citation needed]
Studies of brain structure
A number of sections of the brain have been reported to be sexually dimorphic; that is, they vary between men and women. There have also been reports of variations in brain structure corresponding to sexual orientation. In 1990, Dick Swaab and Michel A. Hofman reported a difference in the size of the suprachiasmatic nucleus between homosexual and heterosexual men.[70] In 1992, Allen and Gorski reported a difference related to sexual orientation in the size of the anterior commissure,[71] but this research was refuted by numerous studies, one of which found that the entirety of the variation was caused by a single outlier.[72][73][74]
Research on the physiologic differences between male and female brains are based on the idea that people have male or a female brain, and this mirrors the behavioral differences between the two sexes. Some researchers state that solid scientific support for this is lacking. Although consistent differences have been identified, including the size of the brain and of specific brain regions, male and female brains are very similar.[75][76][77][78][79][80]
Sexually dimorphic nuclei in the anterior hypothalamus
LeVay also conducted some of these early researches. He studied four groups of neurons in the hypothalamus called INAH1, INAH2, INAH3 and INAH4. This was a relevant area of the brain to study, because of evidence that it played a role in the regulation of sexual behaviour in animals, and because INAH2 and INAH3 had previously been reported to differ in size between men and women.[50]
He obtained brains from 41 deceased hospital patients. The subjects were classified into three groups. The first group comprised 19 gay men who had died of AIDS-related illnesses. The second group comprised 16 men whose sexual orientation was unknown, but whom the researchers presumed to be heterosexual. Six of these men had died of AIDS-related illnesses. The third group was of six women whom the researchers presumed to be heterosexual. One of the women had died of an AIDS-related illness.[50]
The HIV-positive people in the presumably heterosexual patient groups were all identified from medical records as either intravenous drug abusers or recipients of blood transfusions. Two of the men who identified as heterosexual specifically denied ever engaging in a homosexual sex act. The records of the remaining heterosexual subjects contained no information about their sexual orientation; they were assumed to have been primarily or exclusively heterosexual "on the basis of the numerical preponderance of heterosexual men in the population".[50]
LeVay found no evidence for a difference between the groups in the size of INAH1, INAH2 or INAH4. However, the INAH3 group appeared to be twice as big in the heterosexual male group as in the gay male group; the difference was highly significant, and remained significant when only the six AIDS patients were included in the heterosexual group. The size of INAH3 in the homosexual men's brains was comparable to the size of INAH3 in the heterosexual women's brains.[citation needed]
William Byne and colleagues attempted to identify the size differences reported in INAH 1–4 by replicating the experiment using brain sample from other subjects: 14 HIV-positive homosexual males, 34 presumed heterosexual males (10 HIV-positive), and 34 presumed heterosexual females (9 HIV-positive). The researchers found a significant difference in INAH3 size between heterosexual men and heterosexual women. The INAH3 size of the homosexual men was apparently smaller than that of the heterosexual men, and larger than that of the heterosexual women, though neither difference quite reached statistical significance.[73]
Byne and colleagues also weighed and counted numbers of neurons in INAH3 tests not carried out by LeVay. The results for INAH3 weight were similar to those for INAH3 size; that is, the INAH3 weight for the heterosexual male brains was significantly larger than for the heterosexual female brains, while the results for the gay male group were between those of the other two groups but not quite significantly different from either. The neuron count also found a male-female difference in INAH3, but found no trend related to sexual orientation.[73]
LeVay has said that Byne replicated his work, but that he employed a two-tailed statistical analysis, which is typically reserved for when no previous findings had employed the difference. LeVay has said that "given that my study had already reported a INAH3 to be smaller in gay men, a one tailed approach would have been more appropriate, and it would have yielded a significant difference [between heterosexual and homosexual men]".[13]:110
J. Michael Bailey has criticized LeVay's critics—describing the claim that the INAH-3 difference could be attributable to AIDS as "aggravating", since the "INAH-3 did not differ between the brains of straight men who died of AIDS and those who did not have the disease".[81]:120 Bailey has further criticized the second objection that was raised, that being gay might have somehow caused the difference in INAH-3, and not vice-versa, saying "the problem with this idea is that the hypothalamus appears to develop early. Not a single expert I have ever asked about LeVay's study thought it was plausible that sexual behavior caused the INAH-3 differences."[81]:120
The SCN of homosexual males has been demonstrated to be larger (both the volume and the number of neurons are twice as many as in heterosexual males). These areas of the hypothalamus have not yet been explored in homosexual females nor bisexual males nor females. Although the functional implications of such findings still have not been examined in detail, they cast serious doubt over the widely accepted Dörner hypothesis that homosexual males have a "female hypothalamus" and that the key mechanism of differentiating the "male brain from originally female brain" is the epigenetic influence of testosterone during prenatal development.[82]
A 2010 study by Garcia-Falgueras and Swaab stated that "the fetal brain develops during the intrauterine period in the male direction through a direct action of testosterone on the developing nerve cells, or in the female direction through the absence of this hormone surge. In this way, our gender identity (the conviction of belonging to the male or female gender) and sexual orientation are programmed or organized into our brain structures when we are still in the womb. There is no indication that social environment after birth has an effect on gender identity or sexual orientation."[83]
Ovine model
The domestic ram is used as an experimental model to study early programming of the neural mechanisms which underlie homosexuality, developing from the observation that approximately 8% of domestic rams are sexually attracted to other rams (male-oriented) when compared to the majority of rams which are female-oriented. In many species, a prominent feature of sexual differentiation is the presence of a sexually dimorphic nucleus (SDN) in the preoptic hypothalamus, which is larger in males than in females.
Roselli et al. discovered an ovine SDN (oSDN) in the preoptic hypothalamus that is smaller in male-oriented rams than in female-oriented rams, but similar in size to the oSDN of females. Neurons of the oSDN show aromatase expression which is also smaller in male-oriented rams versus female-oriented rams, suggesting that sexual orientation is neurologically hard-wired and may be influenced by hormones. However, results failed to associate the role of neural aromatase in the sexual differentiation of brain and behavior in the sheep, due to the lack of defeminization of adult sexual partner preference or oSDN volume as a result of aromatase activity in the brain of the fetuses during the critical period. Having said this, it is more likely that oSDN morphology and homosexuality may be programmed through an androgen receptor that does not involve aromatisation. Most of the data suggests that homosexual rams, like female-oriented rams, are masculinized and defeminized with respect to mounting, receptivity, and gonadotrophin secretion, but are not defeminized for sexual partner preferences, also suggesting that such behaviors may be programmed differently. Although the exact function of the oSDN is not fully known, its volume, length, and cell number seem to correlate with sexual orientation, and a dimorphism in its volume and of cells could bias the processing cues involved in partner selection. More research is needed in order to understand the requirements and timing of the development of the oSDN and how prenatal programming effects the expression of mate choice in adulthood.[84]
Childhood gender nonconformity
Childhood gender nonconformity is a strong predictor of adult sexual orientation that has been consistently replicated in research, and is thought to be strong evidence of a biological difference between heterosexual and non-heterosexuals. A review authored by J. Michael Bailey states: "childhood gender nonconformity comprises the following phenomena among boys: cross-dressing, desiring to have long hair, playing with dolls, disliking competitive sports and rough play, preferring girls as playmates, exhibiting elevated separation anxiety, and desiring to be—or believing that one is—a girl. In girls, gender nonconformity comprises dressing like and playing with boys, showing interest in competitive sports and rough play, lacking interest in conventionally female toys such as dolls and makeup, and desiring to be a boy". This gender nonconformist behavior typically emerges at preschool age, although is often evident as early as age 2. Children are only considered gender nonconforming if they persistently engage in a variety of these behaviors, as opposed to engaging in a behavior on a few times or on occasion. It is also not a one-dimensional trait, but rather has varying degrees.[4]
Children who grow up to be non-heterosexual were, on average, substantially more gender nonconforming in childhood. This is confirmed in both retrospective studies where homosexuals, bisexuals and heterosexuals are asked about their gender typical behavior in childhood, and in prospective studies, where highly gender nonconforming children are followed from childhood into adulthood to find out their sexual orientation. A review of retrospective studies that measured gender nonconforming traits estimated that 89% of homosexual men exceeded heterosexual males level of gender nonconformity, whereas just 2% of heterosexual men exceeded the homosexual median. For female sexual orientation, the figures were 81% and 12% respectively. A variety of other assessments such as childhood home videos, photos and reports of parents also confirm this finding.[4] Critics of this research see this as confirming stereotypes; however, no study has ever demonstrated that this research has exaggerated childhood gender nonconformity. J. Michael Bailey argues that gay men often deny that they were gender nonconforming in childhood because they may have been bullied or maltreated by peers and parents for it, and because they often do not find femininity attractive in other gay males and thus would not want to acknowledge it in themselves.[85] Additional research in Western cultures and non-Western cultures including Latin America, Asia, Polynesia, and the Middle East supports the validity of childhood gender nonconformity as a predictor of adult non-heterosexuality.[4]
This research does not mean that all non-heterosexuals were gender nonconforming, but rather indicates that long before sexual attraction is known, non-heterosexuals, on average, are noticeably different from other children. There is little evidence that gender nonconforming children have been encouraged or taught to behave that way; rather, childhood gender nonconformity typically emerges despite conventional socialization.[4] Medical experiments in which infant boys were sex reassigned and reared as girls did not make them feminine nor attracted to males.[4]
Boys who were surgically reassigned female
Between the 1960s and 2000, many newborn and infant boys were surgically reassigned as females if they were born with malformed penises, or if they lost their penises in accidents.[4]:72–73 Many surgeons believed such males would be happier being socially and surgically reassigned female. In all seven published cases that have provided sexual orientation information, the subjects grew up to be attracted to females. Six cases were exclusively attracted to females, with one case 'predominantly' attracted to females. In a review article in the journal Psychological Science in the Public Interest, six researchers including J. Michael Bailey state this establishes a strong case that male sexual orientation is partly established before birth:
This is the result we would expect if male sexual orientation were entirely due to nature, and it is opposite of the result expected if it were due to nurture, in which case we would expect that none of these individuals would be predominantly attracted to women. They show how difficult it is to derail the development of male sexual orientation by psychosocial means.
They further argue that this raises questions about the significance of the social environment on sexual orientation, stating, "If one cannot reliably make a male human become attracted to other males by cutting off his penis in infancy and rearing him as a girl, then what other psychosocial intervention could plausibly have that effect?" It is further stated that neither cloacal exstrophy (resulting in a malformed penis), nor surgical accidents, are associated with abnormalities of prenatal androgens, thus, the brains of these individuals were male-organized at birth. Six of the seven identified as heterosexual males at follow up, despite being surgically altered and reared as females, with researchers adding: "available evidence indicates that in such instances, parents are deeply committed to raising these children as girls and in as gender-typical a manner as possible." Bailey et al. describe these sex reassignments as 'the near-perfect quasi-experiment' in measuring the impact of 'nature' versus 'nurture' with regards to male homosexuality.[4]
'Exotic becomes erotic' theory
Daryl Bem, a social psychologist at Cornell University, has theorized that the influence of biological factors on sexual orientation may be mediated by experiences in childhood. A child's temperament predisposes the child to prefer certain activities over others. Because of their temperament, which is influenced by biological variables such as genetic factors, some children will be attracted to activities that are commonly enjoyed by other children of the same gender. Others will prefer activities that are typical of another gender. This will make a gender-conforming child feel different from opposite-gender children, while gender-nonconforming children will feel different from children of their own gender. According to Bem, this feeling of difference will evoke psychological arousal when the child is near members of the gender which it considers as being 'different'. Bem theorizes that this psychological arousal will later be transformed into sexual arousal: children will become sexually attracted to the gender which they see as different ("exotic"). This proposal is known as the "exotic becomes erotic" theory.[86] Wetherell et al. state that Bem "does not intend his model as an absolute prescription for all individuals, but rather as a modal or average explanation."[87]
Two critiques of Bem's theory in the journal Psychological Review concluded that "studies cited by Bem and additional research show that [the] Exotic Becomes Erotic theory is not supported by scientific evidence."[88] Bem was criticized for relying on a non-random sample of gay men from the 1970s (rather than collecting new data) and for drawing conclusions that appear to contradict the original data. An "examination of the original data showed virtually all respondents were familiar with children of both sexes", and that only 9% of gay men said that "none or only a few" of their friends were male, and most gay men (74%) reported having "an especially close friend of the same sex" during grade school.[88] Further, "71% of gay men reported feeling different from other boys, but so did 38% of heterosexual men. The difference for gay men is larger, but still indicates that feeling different from same-sex peers was common for heterosexual men." Bem also acknowledged that gay men were more likely to have older brothers (the fraternal birth order effect), which appeared to contradict an unfamiliarity with males. Bem cited cross-cultural studies which also "appear to contradict the EBE theory assertion", such as the Sambia tribe in Papua New Guinea, which ritually enforced homosexual acts among teenagers; yet once these boys reached adulthood, only a small proportion of men continued to engage in homosexual behaviour - similar to levels observed in the United States.[88] Additionally, Bem's model could be interpreted as implying that if one could change a child's behavior, one could change their sexual orientation, but most psychologists doubt this would be possible.[89]
Neuroscientist Simon LeVay said that while Bem's theory was arranged in a "believable temporal order",[13]:65 that it ultimately "lacks empirical support".[13]:164 Social psychologist Justin Lehmiller stated that Bem's theory has received praise "for the way it seamlessly links biological and environmental influences" and that there "is also some support for the model in the sense that childhood gender nonconformity is indeed one of the strongest predictors of adult homosexuality", but that the validity of the model "has been questioned on numerous grounds and scientists have largely rejected it."[89]
Sexual orientation and evolution
General
Sexual practices that significantly reduce the frequency of heterosexual intercourse also significantly decrease the chances of successful reproduction, and for this reason, they would appear to be maladaptive in an evolutionary context following a simple Darwinian model (competition amongst individuals) of natural selection—on the assumption that homosexuality would reduce this frequency. Several theories have been advanced to explain this contradiction, and new experimental evidence has demonstrated their feasibility.[90]
Some scholars[90] have suggested that homosexuality is indirectly adaptive, by conferring a reproductive advantage in a non-obvious way on heterosexual siblings or their children, a hypothesised instance of kin selection. By way of analogy, the allele (a particular version of a gene) which causes sickle-cell anemia when two copies are present, also confers resistance to malaria with a lesser form of anemia when one copy is present (this is called heterozygous advantage).[91]
Brendan Zietsch of the Queensland Institute of Medical Research proposes the alternative theory that men exhibiting female traits become more attractive to females and are thus more likely to mate, provided the genes involved do not drive them to complete rejection of heterosexuality.[92]
In a 2008 study, its authors stated that "There is considerable evidence that human sexual orientation is genetically influenced, so it is not known how homosexuality, which tends to lower reproductive success, is maintained in the population at a relatively high frequency." They hypothesized that "while genes predisposing to homosexuality reduce homosexuals' reproductive success, they may confer some advantage in heterosexuals who carry them". Their results suggested that "genes predisposing to homosexuality may confer a mating advantage in heterosexuals, which could help explain the evolution and maintenance of homosexuality in the population".[93] However, in the same study, the authors noted that "nongenetic alternative explanations cannot be ruled out" as a reason for the heterosexual in the homosexual-heterosexual twin pair having more partners, specifically citing "social pressure on the other twin to act in a more heterosexual way" (and thus seek out a greater number of sexual partners) as an example of one alternative explanation. The study acknowledges that a large number of sexual partners may not lead to greater reproductive success, specifically noting there is an "absence of evidence relating the number of sexual partners and actual reproductive success, either in the present or in our evolutionary past".[93]
The heterosexual advantage hypothesis was given strong support by the 2004 Italian study demonstrating increased fecundity in the female matrilineal relatives of gay men.[66][67] As originally pointed out by Hamer,[94] even a modest increase in reproductive capacity in females carrying a "gay gene" could easily account for its maintenance at high levels in the population.[67]
In October 2023, biologists reported studies of animals (over 1,500 different species) that found same-sex behavior (not necessarily related to human orientation) may help improve social stability by reducing conflict within the groups studied.[95][96]
Gay uncle hypothesis
The "gay uncle hypothesis" posits that people who themselves do not have children may nonetheless increase the prevalence of their family's genes in future generations by providing resources (e.g., food, supervision, defense, shelter) to the offspring of their closest relatives.[97]
This hypothesis is an extension of the theory of kin selection, which was originally developed to explain apparent altruistic acts which seemed to be maladaptive. The initial concept was suggested by J. B. S. Haldane in 1932 and later elaborated by many others including John Maynard Smith, W. D. Hamilton and Mary Jane West-Eberhard.[98] This concept was also used to explain the patterns of certain social insects where most of the members are non-reproductive.
Vasey and VanderLaan (2010) tested the theory on the Pacific island of Samoa, where they studied women, straight men, and the fa'afafine, men who prefer other men as sexual partners and are accepted within the culture as a distinct third gender category. Vasey and VanderLaan found that the fa'afafine said they were significantly more willing to help kin, yet much less interested in helping children who are not family, providing the first evidence to support the kin selection hypothesis.[99][100]
The hypothesis is consistent with other studies on homosexuality, which show that it is more prevalent amongst both siblings and twins.[99][100]
Vasey and VanderLaan (2011) provides evidence that if an adaptively designed avuncular male androphilic phenotype exists and its development is contingent on a particular social environment, then a collectivistic cultural context is insufficient, in and of itself, for the expression of such a phenotype.[101]
Biological differences in gay men and lesbian women
Some studies have found correlations between physiology of people and their sexuality; these studies provide evidence which suggests that:
- Gay men and straight women have, on average, equally proportioned brain hemispheres. Lesbian women and straight men have, on average, slightly larger right brain hemispheres.[102]
- The suprachiasmatic nucleus of the hypothalamus was found by Swaab and Hopffman to be larger in gay men than in non-gay men;[103] the suprachiasmatic nucleus is also known to be larger in men than in women.[104][105]
- Gay men report, on average, slightly longer and thicker penises than non-gay men.[106]
- The average size of the INAH 3 in the brains of gay men is approximately the same size as INAH 3 in women, which is significantly smaller, and the cells more densely packed, than in heterosexual men's brains.[50]
- The anterior commissure was found to be larger in gay men than women and heterosexual men,[71] but a subsequent study found no such difference.[107]
- The functioning of the inner ear and the central auditory system in lesbians and bisexual women are more like the functional properties found in men than in non-gay women (the researchers argued this finding was consistent with the prenatal hormonal theory of sexual orientation).[108]
- The startle response (eyeblink following a loud sound) is similarly masculinized in lesbians and bisexual women.[109]
- Gay and non-gay people's brains respond differently to two putative sex pheromones (AND, found in male armpit secretions, and EST, found in female urine).[68][110][111]
- The amygdala, a region of the brain, is more active in gay men than non-gay men when exposed to sexually arousing material.[112]
- Finger length ratios between the index and ring fingers have been reported to differ, on average, between non-gay and lesbian women.[113][114][115][116][117][118][119][120][121][122]
- Gay men and lesbians are significantly more likely to be left-handed or ambidextrous than non-gay men and women;[123][124][125] Simon LeVay argues that because "[h]and preference is observable before birth...[126] [t]he observation of increased non-right-handness in gay people is therefore consistent with the idea that sexual orientation is influenced by prenatal processes," perhaps heredity.[50]
- A study of over 50 gay men found that about 23% had counterclockwise hair whorl, as opposed to 8% in the general population. This may correlate with left-handedness.[127]
- Gay men have increased ridge density in the fingerprints on their left thumbs and little fingers.[127]
- Length of limbs and hands of gay men is smaller compared to height than the general population, but only among white men.[127]
J. Michael Bailey has argued that the early childhood gender nonconforming behavior of homosexuals, as opposed to biological markers, are better evidence of homosexuality being an inborn trait. He argues that gay men are "punished much more than rewarded" for their childhood gender nonconformity, and that such behavior "emerges with no encouragement, and despite opposition", making it "the sine qua non of innateness".[128]
Political aspects
Whether genetic or other physiological or psychological determinants form the basis of sexual orientation is a highly politicized issue. The Advocate, a U.S. gay and lesbian newsmagazine, reported in 1996 that 61% of its readers believed that "it would mostly help gay and lesbian rights if homosexuality were found to be biologically determined".[129] A cross-national study in the United States , the Philippines , and Sweden found that those who believed that "homosexuals are born that way" held significantly more positive attitudes toward homosexuality than those who believed that "homosexuals choose to be that way" or "learn to be that way".[130][131]
Equal protection analysis in U.S. law determines when government requirements create a "suspect classification" of groups and therefore eligible for heightened scrutiny based on several factors, one of which is immutability.[132]
Evidence that sexual orientation is biologically determined (and therefore perhaps immutable in the legal sense) would strengthen the legal case for heightened scrutiny of laws discriminating on that basis.[133][134][135]
The perceived causes of sexual orientation have a significant bearing on the status of sexual minorities in the eyes of social conservatives. The Family Research Council, a conservative Christian think tank in Washington, D.C., argues in the book Getting It Straight that finding people are born gay "would advance the idea that sexual orientation is an innate characteristic, like race; that homosexuals, like African-Americans, should be legally protected against 'discrimination;' and that disapproval of homosexuality should be as socially stigmatized as racism. However, it is not true." On the other hand, some social conservatives such as Reverend Robert Schenck have argued that people can accept any scientific evidence while still morally opposing homosexuality.[136] National Organization for Marriage board member and fiction writer Orson Scott Card has supported biological research on homosexuality, writing that "our scientific efforts in regard to homosexuality should be to identify genetic and uterine causes... so that the incidence of this dysfunction can be minimized.... [However, this should not be seen] as an attack on homosexuals, a desire to 'commit genocide' against the homosexual community... There is no 'cure' for homosexuality because it is not a disease. There are, however, different ways of living with homosexual desires."[137]
Some advocates for the rights of sexual minorities resist what they perceive as attempts to pathologise or medicalise 'deviant' sexuality, and choose to fight for acceptance in a moral or social realm.[136] The journalist Chandler Burr has stated that "[s]ome, recalling earlier psychiatric "treatments" for homosexuality, discern in the biological quest the seeds of genocide. They conjure up the specter of the surgical or chemical "rewiring" of gay people, or of abortions of fetal homosexuals who have been hunted down in the womb."[138] LeVay has said in response to letters from gays and lesbians making such criticisms that the research "has contributed to the status of gay people in society".[136]
LGBT population estimates by country
Country | LGBT population estimates |
---|---|
010 China
|
40,000,000–70,000,000[139][140] |
037 India
|
2,500,000–70,000,000[141][142] |
048 United States
|
20,000,000–30,000,000[143][144] |
048 Indonesia
|
12,700,000[145] |
048 Brazil
|
8,000,000–20,000,000[146][147] |
048 Nigeria
|
6,900,000–10,400,000[148] |
048 Japan
|
7,100,000[149] |
048 Mexico
|
6,000,000[150] |
048 Philippines
|
4,900,000[151] |
048 Vietnam
|
4,700,000[152] |
048 Germany
|
4,600,000[153] |
048 France
|
3,600,000-4,900,000[154][155] |
048 Thailand
|
3,600,000[156] |
048 United Kingdom
|
3,500,000-4,400,000[157][158] |
048 Italy
|
3,400,000[159] |
048 Spain
|
2,600,000[160] |
048 Colombia
|
2,500,000[161] |
048 Argentina
|
2,200,000[162] |
048 Poland
|
2,100,000[163] |
048 Peru
|
1,500,000[164] |
048 Australia
|
1,400,000[165] |
048 Taiwan
|
1,300,000[166] |
See also
- Against Nature?
- Causes of gender incongruence
- Environment and sexual orientation
- Epigenetic theories of homosexuality
- Gay bomb
- Homosexual behavior in animals
- Neuroscience and sexual orientation
- Norms of reaction
References
- ↑ 1.0 1.1 "Sexual orientation and adolescents". Pediatrics 113 (6): 1827–32. June 2004. doi:10.1542/peds.113.6.1827. PMID 15173519.
- ↑ Marriages, Families, and Relationships: Making Choices in a Diverse Society. Cengage Learning. 2014. p. 82. ISBN 978-1305176898. https://books.google.com/books?id=fofaAgAAQBAJ&pg=PA82. Retrieved February 11, 2016. "The reason some individuals develop a gay sexual identity has not been definitively established – nor do we yet understand the development of heterosexuality. The American Psychological Association (APA) takes the position that a variety of factors impact a person's sexuality. The most recent literature from the APA says that sexual orientation is not a choice that can be changed at will, and that sexual orientation is most likely the result of a complex interaction of environmental, cognitive and biological factors...is shaped at an early age...[and evidence suggests] biological, including inborn hormonal factors which play a significant role in a person's sexuality (American Psychological Association 2010)."
- ↑ Principles and Practice of Psychiatric Nursing. Elsevier Health Sciences. 2014. p. 502. ISBN 978-0323294126. https://books.google.com/books?id=ivALBAAAQBAJ&pg=PA502. Retrieved February 11, 2016. "No conclusive evidence supports any one specific cause of homosexuality; however, most researchers agree that biological and social factors influence the development of sexual orientation."
- ↑ 4.00 4.01 4.02 4.03 4.04 4.05 4.06 4.07 4.08 4.09 4.10 4.11 4.12 4.13 4.14 4.15 4.16 4.17 "Sexual Orientation, Controversy, and Science". Psychological Science in the Public Interest 17 (2): 45–101. September 2016. doi:10.1177/1529100616637616. PMID 27113562.
- ↑ 5.0 5.1 "Minireview: Hormones and human sexual orientation". Endocrinology 152 (8): 2937–47. August 2011. doi:10.1210/en.2011-0277. PMID 21693676.
- ↑ Balthazart, Jacques (August 2011). "Minireview: Hormones and Human Sexual Orientation". Endocrinology 152 (8): 2937–2947. doi:10.1210/en.2011-0277. PMID 21693676.
- ↑ 7.0 7.1 7.2 7.3 "Prenatal Influences on Human Sexual Orientation: Expectations versus Data". Archives of Sexual Behavior 46 (6): 1583–1592. August 2017. doi:10.1007/s10508-016-0904-2. PMID 28176027.
- ↑ 8.0 8.1 8.2 "Neurobiology of gender identity and sexual orientation". Journal of Neuroendocrinology 30 (7): e12562. July 2018. doi:10.1111/jne.12562. PMID 29211317.
- ↑ 9.0 9.1 9.2 "Fraternal birth order effect on sexual orientation explained". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 115 (2): 234–236. January 2018. doi:10.1073/pnas.1719534115. PMID 29259109. Bibcode: 2018PNAS..115..234B.
- ↑ 10.0 10.1 "Male homosexuality and maternal immune responsivity to the Y-linked protein NLGN4Y". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 115 (2): 302–306. January 2018. doi:10.1073/pnas.1705895114. PMID 29229842. Bibcode: 2018PNAS..115..302B.
- ↑ "The Fraternal Birth Order Effect Is Robust and Important". Archives of Sexual Behavior 47 (1): 17–19. January 2018. doi:10.1007/s10508-017-1115-1. PMID 29159754.
- ↑ "Fraternal Birth Order, Family Size, and Male Homosexuality: Meta-Analysis of Studies Spanning 25 Years". Archives of Sexual Behavior 47 (1): 1–15. January 2018. doi:10.1007/s10508-017-1007-4. PMID 28608293.
- ↑ 13.0 13.1 13.2 13.3 13.4 13.5 13.6 "The Brain" (in en). Gay, Straight, and the Reason Why: The Science of Sexual Orientation. Oxford University Press. August 2016. ISBN 978-0-19-029739-8. https://books.google.com/books?id=wnHADAAAQBAJ&pg=PA108.
- ↑ Zhou, Jiang-Ning; Hofman, Michel A.; Gooren, Louis J. G.; Swaab, Dick F. (November 1995). "A sex difference in the human brain and its relation to transsexuality". Nature 378 (6552): 68–70. doi:10.1038/378068a0. PMID 7477289.
- ↑ 15.0 15.1 "Homosexuality as a consequence of epigenetically canalized sexual development". The Quarterly Review of Biology 87 (4): 343–68. December 2012. doi:10.1086/668167. PMID 23397798. https://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1086/668167.
- ↑ "Understanding Homosexuality: Moving on from Patterns to Mechanisms". Archives of Sexual Behavior 47 (1): 27–31. January 2018. doi:10.1007/s10508-017-1092-4. PMID 28986707. https://www.gwern.net/docs/psychology/2017-gavrilets.pdf.
- ↑ "Homosexuality via canalized sexual development: a testing protocol for a new epigenetic model". BioEssays 35 (9): 764–70. September 2013. doi:10.1002/bies.201300033. PMID 23868698.
- ↑ "Maternal Thyroid Dysfunction During Pregnancy May Lead to Same-sex Attraction/gender Nonconformity in the Offspring: Proposal of Prenatal Thyroid Model". European Psychiatry 30: 374. March 2015. doi:10.1016/s0924-9338(15)30294-7. ISSN 0924-9338.
- ↑ "High Rates of Same-Sex Attraction/Gender Nonconformity in the Offspring of Mothers with Thyroid Dysfunction During Pregnancy: Proposal of Prenatal Thyroid Model". Mental Illness 7 (2): 5810. September 2015. doi:10.4081/mi.2015.5810. PMID 26605033.
- ↑ "Towards a further understanding of prenatal thyroid theory of homosexuality: Autoimmune thyroiditis, polycystic ovary syndrome, autism and low birth weight". Mental Illness 9 (2): 7325. October 2017. doi:10.4081/mi.2017.7325. PMID 29142667.
- ↑ "A second group of youngsters with gender nonconformity/same-sex attraction born to mothers with thyroid dysfunction in pregnancy". 30 August 2022. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/326723698.
- ↑ "Fetal exposure to prescription drugs and adult sexual orientation". Personality and Individual Differences 38 (1): 225–236. January 2005. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.04.004. ISSN 0191-8869.
- ↑ "Same-sex marriage, autoimmune thyroid gland dysfunction and other autoimmune diseases in Denmark 1989-2008". European Journal of Epidemiology 29 (1): 63–71. January 2014. doi:10.1007/s10654-013-9869-9. PMID 24306355.
- ↑ "A Link Between Maternal Thyroid Hormone and Sexual Orientation?". Mental Illness 8 (1): 6591. May 2016. doi:10.4081/mi.2016.6591. PMID 27403279.
- ↑ 25.0 25.1 "Thyroid hormone receptors and ligands, tissue distribution and sexual behavior". Molecular and Cellular Endocrinology 467: 49–59. May 2018. doi:10.1016/j.mce.2017.11.006. PMID 29175529.
- ↑ "Born This Way: Using Intrinsic Disorder to Map the Connections between SLITRKs, TSHR, and Male Sexual Orientation". Proteomics 18 (21–22): e1800307. November 2018. doi:10.1002/pmic.201800307. PMID 30156382.
- ↑ "The biological basis of sexual orientation: How hormonal, genetic, and environmental factors influence to whom we are sexually attracted". Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 55: 100798. October 2019. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2019.100798. PMID 31593707.
- ↑ "Thyroid diseases and gender". Italian Journal of Gender-Specific Medicine 5 (September–December): 136–141. September 2019. doi:10.1723/3245.32148.
- ↑ "Sexual Identity and Sexual Orientation". Hormones, Brain and Behavior (Third ed.). Elsevier. 2017. pp. 279–290. ISBN 978-1-78684-205-3. OCLC 971456116.
- ↑ 30.0 30.1 30.2 "Genome-Wide Association Study of Male Sexual Orientation" (in En). Scientific Reports 7 (1): 16950. December 2017. doi:10.1038/s41598-017-15736-4. PMID 29217827. Bibcode: 2017NatSR...716950S.
- ↑ "A genetic study of male sexual orientation". Archives of General Psychiatry 48 (12): 1089–96. December 1991. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1991.01810360053008. PMID 1845227.
- ↑ "Homosexual orientation in twins: a report on 61 pairs and three triplet sets". Archives of Sexual Behavior 22 (3): 187–206. June 1993. doi:10.1007/bf01541765. PMID 8494487.
- ↑ "Genetic and environmental influences on sexual orientation and its correlates in an Australian twin sample". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78 (3): 524–36. March 2000. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.3.524. PMID 10743878.
- ↑ "Biological Factors in the Development of Sexual Orientation". Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Identities and Youth: Psychological Perspectives. Oxford [England]: Oxford University Press. 2001. pp. 27–51. ISBN 978-0-19-511953-4. https://books.google.com/books?id=zOXwENojFHYC&pg=PA27. quoted in "Opposite-sex twins and adolescent same-sex attraction.". American Journal of Sociology 107 (5): 1179–1205. March 2002. doi:10.1086/341906.
- ↑ 35.0 35.1 "Genetic and environmental effects on same-sex sexual behavior: a population study of twins in Sweden". Archives of Sexual Behavior 39 (1): 75–80. February 2010. doi:10.1007/s10508-008-9386-1. PMID 18536986.
- ↑ "A linkage between DNA markers on the X chromosome and male sexual orientation". Science 261 (5119): 321–7. July 1993. doi:10.1126/science.8332896. PMID 8332896. Bibcode: 1993Sci...261..321H. https://zenodo.org/record/1231257.
- ↑ 37.0 37.1 (in en) Born Gay: The Psychobiology of Sex Orientation (2nd ed.). Peter Owen Publishers. 2008. ISBN 9780720613094.
- ↑ 38.0 38.1 "Linkage between sexual orientation and chromosome Xq28 in males but not in females". Nature Genetics 11 (3): 248–56. November 1995. doi:10.1038/ng1195-248. PMID 7581447. https://zenodo.org/record/1233407.
- ↑ "Genetics of sexual development". Annual Review of Sex Research 11: 1–25. 2000. PMID 11351829.
- ↑ "Genetics and Male Sexual Orientation". Science 285 (5429): 803. 1999. doi:10.1126/science.285.5429.803a.
- ↑ "A genomewide scan of male sexual orientation". Human Genetics 116 (4): 272–8. March 2005. doi:10.1007/s00439-004-1241-4. PMID 15645181. http://mypage.iu.edu/~bmustans/Mustanski_etal_2005.pdf.
- ↑ 42.0 42.1 "Genome-wide scan demonstrates significant linkage for male sexual orientation". Psychological Medicine 45 (7): 1379–88. May 2015. doi:10.1017/S0033291714002451. PMID 25399360.
- ↑ The Biological Basis of Human Sexual Orientation: Is There a Role for Epigenetics?. Advances in Genetics. 86. 2014. pp. 167–84. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-800222-3.00008-5. ISBN 9780128002223. http://gaystudies.genetics.ucla.edu/UCLA%20Twin%20Sexual%20Orientation%20Study/Read%20More_files/Ngun.pdf. Retrieved 20 March 2016.
- ↑ "Eye color, hair color, blood type, and the rhesus factor: exploring possible genetic links to sexual orientation". Archives of Sexual Behavior 37 (1): 145–9. February 2008. doi:10.1007/s10508-007-9274-0. PMID 18074215.
- ↑ Animal Homosexuality: A Biosocial Perspective. Cambridge University Press. 2010. pp. 55–96. ISBN 978-1139490382. https://books.google.com/books?id=EftT_1bsPOAC.
- ↑ "Courtship behavior in Drosophila melanogaster: towards a 'courtship connectome'". Current Opinion in Neurobiology 23 (1): 76–83. February 2013. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2012.09.002. PMID 23021897.
- ↑ "Male-like sexual behavior of female mouse lacking fucose mutarotase". BMC Genetics 11: 62. July 2010. doi:10.1186/1471-2156-11-62. PMID 20609214.
- ↑ "The 'gay gene' is back on the scene". 31 October 1995. https://www.independent.co.uk/news/the-gay-gene-is-back-on-the-scene-1536770.html.
- ↑ "Being homosexual is only partly due to gay gene, research finds". Telegraph Media Group. 13 February 2014. https://www.telegraph.co.uk/science/science-news/10637532/Being-homosexual-is-only-partly-due-to-gay-gene-research-finds.html.
- ↑ 50.0 50.1 50.2 50.3 50.4 50.5 "A difference in hypothalamic structure between heterosexual and homosexual men". Science 253 (5023): 1034–7. August 1991. doi:10.1126/science.1887219. PMID 1887219. Bibcode: 1991Sci...253.1034L.
- ↑ "Same-sex marriage, autoimmune thyroid gland dysfunction and other autoimmune diseases in Denmark 1989-2008". European Journal of Epidemiology 29 (1): 63–71. January 2014. doi:10.1007/s10654-013-9869-9. PMID 24306355.
- ↑ "Determinants of body weight among men of different sexual orientation". Preventive Medicine 51 (2): 129–31. August 2010. doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2010.05.010. PMID 20510272.
- ↑ "Biodemographic comparisons of homosexual and heterosexual men in the Kinsey Interview Data". Archives of Sexual Behavior 25 (6): 551–79. December 1996. doi:10.1007/BF02437839. PMID 8931880.
- ↑ "Giant study links DNA variants to same-sex behavior" (in en). AAAS. 2018-10-19. https://www.science.org/content/article/giant-study-links-dna-variants-same-sex-behavior.
- ↑
- "Large-scale GWAS reveals insights into the genetic architecture of same-sex sexual behavior". Science 365 (6456): eaat7693. August 2019. doi:10.1126/science.aat7693. PMID 31467194.
- "Genetics of Sexual Behavior". geneticsexbehavior.info. 28 February 2018. https://geneticsexbehavior.info/what-we-found/.
- ↑ "Discovery of new genetic loci for male sexual orientation in Han population". Cell Discovery 7 (1): 103. October 2021. doi:10.1038/s41421-021-00341-7. PMID 34719679.
- ↑ "Extreme skewing of X chromosome inactivation in mothers of homosexual men". Human Genetics 118 (6): 691–4. February 2006. doi:10.1007/s00439-005-0119-4. PMID 16369763. http://repositories.cdlib.org/postprints/1413.
- ↑ 58.0 58.1 "H-Y antigen and homosexuality in men". Journal of Theoretical Biology 185 (3): 373–8. April 1997. doi:10.1006/jtbi.1996.0315. PMID 9156085. Bibcode: 1997JThBi.185..373B. http://faculty.bennington.edu/~sherman/sex/H-Y%20anitgen.pdf.
- ↑ "Pas de Deux of Sexuality Is Written in the Genes". The New York Times. 10 April 2007. https://www.nytimes.com/2007/04/10/health/10gene.html.
- ↑ "Birth order and sibling sex ratio in homosexual versus heterosexual males and females". Annual Review of Sex Research 8: 27–67. 1997. PMID 10051890.
- ↑ 61.0 61.1 "Sexual orientation, fraternal birth order, and the maternal immune hypothesis: a review". Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 32 (2): 247–54. April 2011. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2011.02.004. PMID 21315103.
- ↑ "How many gay men owe their sexual orientation to fraternal birth order?". Archives of Sexual Behavior 31 (1): 63–71. February 2002. doi:10.1023/a:1014031201935. PMID 11910793.
- ↑ "Proportion of homosexual men who owe their sexual orientation to fraternal birth order: An estimate based on two national probability samples". American Journal of Human Biology 16 (2): 151–7. 2004. doi:10.1002/ajhb.20006. PMID 14994314.
- ↑ "Fertility in the mothers of firstborn homosexual and heterosexual men". Archives of Sexual Behavior 41 (3): 551–6. June 2012. doi:10.1007/s10508-011-9888-0. PMID 22187029.
- ↑ "Further data concerning Blanchard's (2011) "Fertility in the mothers of firstborn homosexual and heterosexual men"". Archives of Sexual Behavior 41 (3): 529–31. June 2012. doi:10.1007/s10508-012-9942-6. PMID 22399055.
- ↑ 66.0 66.1 "Evidence for maternally inherited factors favouring male homosexuality and promoting female fecundity". Proceedings. Biological Sciences 271 (1554): 2217–21. November 2004. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2872. PMID 15539346.
- ↑ 67.0 67.1 67.2 "Sexually antagonistic selection in human male homosexuality". PLOS ONE 3 (6): e2282. June 2008. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0002282. PMID 18560521. Bibcode: 2008PLoSO...3.2282C.
- ↑ 68.0 68.1 "Brain response to putative pheromones in homosexual men". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 102 (20): 7356–61. May 2005. doi:10.1073/pnas.0407998102. PMID 15883379. Bibcode: 2005PNAS..102.7356S.
- ↑ "Gay Men Are Found to Have Different Scent of Attraction". The New York Times. 9 May 2005. https://www.nytimes.com/2005/05/09/science/09cnd-smell.html.
- ↑ "An enlarged suprachiasmatic nucleus in homosexual men". Brain Research 537 (1–2): 141–8. December 1990. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(90)90350-K. PMID 2085769. https://pure.knaw.nl/portal/files/488706/14928_285_swaab.pdf.
- ↑ 71.0 71.1 "Sexual orientation and the size of the anterior commissure in the human brain". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 89 (15): 7199–202. August 1992. doi:10.1073/pnas.89.15.7199. PMID 1496013. Bibcode: 1992PNAS...89.7199A.
- ↑ "Human sexual orientation. The biologic theories reappraised". Archives of General Psychiatry 50 (3): 228–39. March 1993. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820150078009. PMID 8439245.
- ↑ 73.0 73.1 73.2 "The interstitial nuclei of the human anterior hypothalamus: an investigation of variation with sex, sexual orientation, and HIV status". Hormones and Behavior 40 (2): 86–92. September 2001. doi:10.1006/hbeh.2001.1680. PMID 11534967. [yes|permanent dead link|dead link}}]
- ↑ "A lack of dimorphism of sex or sexual orientation in the human anterior commissure". Brain Research 936 (1–2): 95–8. May 2002. doi:10.1016/s0006-8993(02)02590-8. PMID 11988236.
- ↑ "Why sex matters for neuroscience". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience 7 (6): 477–84. June 2006. doi:10.1038/nrn1909. PMID 16688123.
- ↑ "Sex differences in the adolescent brain". Brain and Cognition 72 (1): 46–55. February 2010. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.008. PMID 19913969.
- ↑ "New tricks by an old dogma: mechanisms of the Organizational/Activational Hypothesis of steroid-mediated sexual differentiation of brain and behavior". Hormones and Behavior 55 (5): 655–65. May 2009. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.02.012. PMID 19682425.
- ↑ "Gonadal steroid action and brain sex differentiation in the rat". Journal of Neuroendocrinology 21 (4): 410–4. March 2009. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2826.2009.01856.x. PMID 19226349.
- ↑ "Brain size, sex, and the aging brain". Human Brain Mapping 36 (1): 150–69. January 2015. doi:10.1002/hbm.22619. PMID 25161056.
- ↑ "How Early Hormones Shape Gender Development". Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 7: 53–60. February 2016. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.11.011. PMID 26688827.
- ↑ 81.0 81.1 The Man Who Would Be Queen. Joseph Henry Press. 2003-03-10. ISBN 978-0-309-08418-5. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/281747420.
- ↑ "Gender and sexual orientation in relation to hypothalamic structures". Hormone Research 38 (Suppl 2): 51–61. 1992. doi:10.1159/000182597. PMID 1292983. https://pure.knaw.nl/portal/en/publications/gender-and-sexual-orientation-in-relation-to-hypothalamic-structures(7cb8b769-4329-407a-b0ee-13e011017f68).html.
- ↑ "Sexual Hormones and the Brain: An Essential Alliance for Sexual Identity and Sexual Orientation". Pediatric Neuroendocrinology. Endocrine Development. 17. 2010. 22–35. doi:10.1159/000262525. ISBN 978-3-8055-9302-1.
- ↑ "Prenatal programming of sexual partner preference: the ram model". Journal of Neuroendocrinology 21 (4): 359–64. March 2009. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2826.2009.01828.x. PMID 19207819.
- ↑ (in en) The Man Who Would Be Queen: The Science of Gender-Bending and Transsexualism. Joseph Henry Press. 2003-03-10. pp. 80. ISBN 978-0-309-08418-5. https://books.google.com/books?id=UhYFNLSGickC&pg=PT96.
- ↑ "Exotic becomes erotic: interpreting the biological correlates of sexual orientation". Archives of Sexual Behavior 29 (6): 531–48. December 2000. doi:10.1023/A:1002050303320. PMID 11100261. http://www.kluweronline.com/art.pdf?issn=0004-0002&volume=29&page=531.[yes|permanent dead link|dead link}}] PDF
- ↑ The SAGE Handbook of Identities. SAGE Publications. 2010. p. 177. ISBN 978-1446248379. https://books.google.com/books?id=BAnvcxmzylQC&pg=PA177.
- ↑ 88.0 88.1 88.2 "A critique of Bem's "Exotic Becomes Erotic" theory of sexual orientation" (PDF). Psychological Review 105 (2): 387–94. April 1998. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.105.2.387. PMID 9577243. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/13707155.
- ↑ 89.0 89.1 (in en) The Psychology of Human Sexuality. John Wiley & Sons. 2017-12-26. pp. 156–157. ISBN 978-1-119-16471-5. https://books.google.com/books?id=I3JGDwAAQBAJ&pg=PA156.
- ↑ 90.0 90.1 "Sperm competition and the persistence of genes for male homosexuality". Bio Systems 31 (2–3): 223–33. 1993. doi:10.1016/0303-2647(93)90051-D. PMID 8155854.
- ↑ Sperm Wars: The Science of Sex (1st ed.). New York: BasicBooks. 1996. p. 241. ISBN 978-0-465-08180-6.
- ↑ "Gender bending". The Economist. 2008-10-23. http://www.economist.com/science/displaystory.cfm?story_id=12465295.
- ↑ 93.0 93.1 "Genetic factors predisposing to homosexuality may increase mating success in heterosexuals". Evolution and Human Behavior 29 (6): 424–433. November 2008. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.07.002.
- ↑ The Science of Desire: The Search for the Gay Gene and the Biology of Behavior. Simon and Schuster. 1994. ISBN 978-0-684-80446-0.
- ↑ Zimmer, Carl (3 October 2023). "Same-Sex Behavior Evolved in Many Mammals to Reduce Conflict, Study Suggests - But the researchers cautioned that the work could not shed much light on sexual orientation in humans. + comment". The New York Times. Archived from the original on 4 October 2023. https://ghostarchive.org/archive/20231004161002/https://www.nytimes.com/2023/10/03/science/same-sex-behavior-evolution-mammals.html#permid=128206761. Retrieved 4 October 2023.
- ↑ Gómez, José M. (3 October 2023). "The evolution of same-sex sexual behaviour in mammals". Nature 14 (5719). doi:10.1038/s41467-023-41290-x. PMC 10547684. Archived from the original on 4 October 2023. https://archive.today/20231004172736/https://www.nature.com/articles/s41467-023-41290-x. Retrieved 4 October 2023.
- ↑ "How Gay Uncles Pass Down Genes" (in en). 11 February 2010. https://www.livescience.com/6106-gay-uncles-pass-genes.html.
- ↑ The growth of biological thought : diversity, evolution, and inheritance. Cambridge, Mass.: Belknap Press. 1982. p. 598. ISBN 978-0-674-36446-2.
- ↑ 99.0 99.1 VanderLaan DP (2011). The development and evolution of male androphilia in Samoan fa'afafine (Ph.D. thesis).
- ↑ 100.0 100.1 "An adaptive cognitive dissociation between willingness to help kin and nonkin in Samoan Fa'afafine". Psychological Science 21 (2): 292–7. February 2010. doi:10.1177/0956797609359623. PMID 20424059.; Lay summary in: "Study Supports Gay Super Uncles Theory". The Advocate. 5 February 2010. http://www.advocate.com/news/daily-news/2010/02/05/study-supports-gay-super-uncles-theory.
- ↑ "Sexual orientation in men and avuncularity in Japan: implications for the kin selection hypothesis". Archives of Sexual Behavior 41 (1): 209–15. February 2012. doi:10.1007/s10508-011-9763-z. PMID 21656333.
- ↑ "Scans see 'gay brain differences'". BBC News - Health. 2008-06-16. http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/health/7456588.stm.
- ↑ "Structural sex differences in the brain: influence of gonadal steroids and behavioral correlates". Journal of Endocrinological Investigation 18 (3): 232–52. March 1995. doi:10.1007/BF03347808. PMID 7615911.
- ↑ "Development of vasoactive intestinal polypeptide neurons in the human suprachiasmatic nucleus in relation to birth and sex". Brain Research. Developmental Brain Research 79 (2): 249–59. June 1994. doi:10.1016/0165-3806(94)90129-5. PMID 7955323.
- ↑ Evolution's Rainbow: Diversity, Gender, and Sexuality in Nature and People. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. 2004. p. 245. ISBN 9780520240735. https://archive.org/details/evolutionsrainbo00roug.
- ↑ "The relation between sexual orientation and penile size". Archives of Sexual Behavior 28 (3): 213–21. June 1999. doi:10.1023/A:1018780108597. PMID 10410197.
- ↑ "A lack of dimorphism of sex or sexual orientation in the human anterior commissure". Brain Research 936 (1–2): 95–8. May 2002. doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(02)02590-8. PMID 11988236.
- ↑ "Masculinization effects in the auditory system". Archives of Sexual Behavior 31 (1): 99–111. February 2002. doi:10.1023/A:1014087319682. PMID 11910797.
- ↑ "Sexual orientation-related differences in prepulse inhibition of the human startle response". Behavioral Neuroscience 117 (5): 1096–102. October 2003. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.117.5.1096. PMID 14570558.
- ↑ "Smelling of odorous sex hormone-like compounds causes sex-differentiated hypothalamic activations in humans". Neuron 31 (4): 661–8. August 2001. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00390-7. PMID 11545724.
- ↑ "Brain response to putative pheromones in lesbian women". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103 (21): 8269–74. May 2006. doi:10.1073/pnas.0600331103. PMID 16705035. Bibcode: 2006PNAS..103.8269B.
- ↑ "Neural correlates of sexual arousal in homosexual and heterosexual men". Behavioral Neuroscience 121 (2): 237–48. April 2007. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.121.2.237. PMID 17469913.. The authors of the study caution that any interpretation of this finding must take into account that the group difference in brain activation between heterosexual men and homosexual men in the amygdala region is not large and that the most robust finding is that both heterosexual and homosexual men used the same areas when they reacted to sexually preferred stimuli. "For the most part, homosexual and heterosexual men showed very similar patterns of activation (albeit to different erotic stimuli). One possible exception was the amygdala, in which homosexual men showed greater activational differences between preferred and nonpreferred erotic stimuli compared with heterosexual men. However, this difference was not hypothesized a priori, was not large, and was the only group difference found out of many tested. Thus, this finding needs replication. "What is Sexual Orientation and do Women Have One?". Contemporary Perspectives on Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Identities. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. 54. 2009. pp. 43–63 (47). doi:10.1007/978-0-387-09556-1_3. ISBN 978-0-387-09555-4.)
- ↑ "Finger-length ratios and sexual orientation". Nature 404 (6777): 455–6. March 2000. doi:10.1038/35006555. PMID 10761903. Bibcode: 2000Natur.404..455W. http://msu.edu/~breedsm/pdf/breedlove2000.pdf.
- ↑ Written on the body: butch vs. femme lesbian gender identity and biological correlates of low digit ratio, Rutgers University, 2002, OCLC 80234273
- ↑ "Finger-length ratios in female monozygotic twins discordant for sexual orientation". Archives of Sexual Behavior 32 (1): 23–8. February 2003. doi:10.1023/A:1021837211630. PMID 12597269.
- ↑ "Sexual orientation and the 2nd to 4th finger length ratio: evidence for organising effects of sex hormones or developmental instability?". Psychoneuroendocrinology 28 (3): 288–303. April 2003. doi:10.1016/S0306-4530(02)00022-7. PMID 12573297.
- ↑ "Sex hormones and finger length: What does 2D:4D indicate?". Evolution and Human Behavior 25 (3): 182–99. May 2004. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.03.005. http://www.anth.ucsb.edu/faculty/gaulin/page1/Puts_et_al_2004.pdf.
- ↑ "Fluctuating asymmetry, second to fourth finger length ratios and human sexual orientation". Psychoneuroendocrinology 30 (4): 382–91. May 2005. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.10.006. PMID 15694118.
- ↑ "Finger length ratio (2D:4D) and dimensions of sexual orientation". Neuropsychobiology 53 (4): 210–4. 2006. doi:10.1159/000094730. PMID 16874008.
- ↑ "2D:4D finger-length ratios in children and adults with gender identity disorder". Hormones and Behavior 54 (3): 450–4. August 2008. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2008.05.002. PMID 18585715.
- ↑ "Sexual orientation and the second to fourth finger length ratio: a meta-analysis in men and women". Behavioral Neuroscience 124 (2): 278–87. April 2010. doi:10.1037/a0018764. PMID 20364887. [yes|permanent dead link|dead link}}]
- ↑ "The second to fourth digit ratio (2D:4D) in a Japanese twin sample: heritability, prenatal hormone transfer, and association with sexual orientation". Archives of Sexual Behavior 41 (3): 711–24. June 2012. doi:10.1007/s10508-011-9889-z. PMID 22270254.
- ↑ "Sexual orientation and handedness in men and women: a meta-analysis". Psychological Bulletin 126 (4): 575–92. July 2000. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.4.575. PMID 10900997.
- ↑ "Dermatoglyphics, handedness, sex, and sexual orientation". Archives of Sexual Behavior 31 (1): 113–22. February 2002. doi:10.1023/A:1014039403752. PMID 11910784.
- ↑ "Handedness, sexual orientation, and gender-related personality traits in men and women". Archives of Sexual Behavior 32 (2): 103–14. April 2003. doi:10.1023/A:1022444223812. PMID 12710825.
- ↑ "Handedness in the human fetus". Neuropsychologia 29 (11): 1107–11. 1991. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(91)90080-R. PMID 1775228.
- ↑ 127.0 127.1 127.2 "The Science of Gaydar". New York Magazine. 18 June 2007. http://nymag.com/news/features/33520/.
- ↑ The Man Who Would Be Queen. Joseph Henry Press. 2003-03-10. pp. 123. ISBN 978-0-309-08418-5. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/281747420.
- ↑ The Advocate (1996, February 6). Advocate Poll Results. p. 8.
- ↑ "Biological explanation, psychological explanation, and tolerance of homosexuals: a cross-national analysis of beliefs and attitudes". Psychological Reports 65 (3 Pt 1): 1003–10. December 1989. doi:10.2466/pr0.1989.65.3.1003. PMID 2608821.
- ↑ "The relationship of heterosexuals' attributions for the causes of homosexuality to attitudes toward lesbians and gay men". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 16 (2): 369–377. 1990. doi:10.1177/0146167290162016.
- ↑ "The Geography of Equal Protection". Minnesota Law Review 101 (4): 1580. 2017. https://www.minnesotalawreview.org/wp-content/uploads/2017/04/Leslie.pdf. Retrieved 2019-12-22. "Thus, because the level of scrutiny is often outcome determinative, the probability of courts protecting gay Americans from discrimination is often a function of whether judges conclude that sexual orientation is a suspect classification. To determine this, courts generally consider four factors: whether the members of the group: (1) have historically been subjected to discrimination; (2) share a defining characteristic unrelated to their ability to perform or contribute to society; (3) share a defining immutable characteristic; and (4) lack political power.".
- ↑ "Equal Protection for Homosexuals: Why the Immutability Argument is Necessary and How it is Met.". Cleveland State Law Review: 545–573. 2005–2006. https://engagedscholarship.csuohio.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?referer=&httpsredir=1&article=1252&context=clevstlrev.
- ↑ "Is Sexuality Immutable?". The New Yorker. 25 January 2010. https://www.newyorker.com/news/news-desk/is-sexuality-immutable.
- ↑ "Prop. 8 trial: defenders of gay-marriage ban make their case". Christian Science Monitor. 26 January 2010. http://www.csmonitor.com/USA/Justice/2010/0125/Prop.-8-trial-defenders-of-gay-marriage-ban-make-their-case. Retrieved 27 January 2010.
- ↑ 136.0 136.1 136.2 "What Makes People Gay?". The Boston Globe. 14 August 2005. http://www.boston.com/news/globe/magazine/articles/2005/08/14/what_makes_people_gay/.
- ↑ "Science on gays falls short". Deseret Morning News. August 7, 2008. http://www.mormontimes.com/content/single/10235.
- ↑ "Homosexuality and Biology". The Atlantic Monthly. June 2007. https://www.theatlantic.com/doc/199706/homosexuality-biology/4.
- ↑ Mavraj, Arber (March 6, 2016). "The LGBT Movement in China: Public Perception, Stigma, and the Human Rights Debate". Inquiries Journal 8 (12). http://www.inquiriesjournal.com/articles/1503/the-lgbt-movement-in-china-public-perception-stigma-and-the-human-rights-debate.
- ↑ Wang, Yuanyuan; Hu, Zhishan; Peng, Ke; Rechdan, Joanne; Yang, Yuan; Wu, Lijuan; Xin, Ying; Lin, Jiahui et al. (2020). "Mapping out a spectrum of the Chinese public's discrimination toward the LGBT community: Results from a national survey". BMC Public Health 20 (1): 669. doi:10.1186/s12889-020-08834-y. PMID 32397988.
- ↑ "In Another Realm". January 26, 2020. https://indianexpress.com/article/india/in-another-realm-lgbtq-community-networking-population-in-india-stigma-around-homosexuality-6235390/.
- ↑ "India's wealthy gays keep niche market in the pink". Reuters. August 10, 2010. https://www.reuters.com/article/india-gay-economy-idINSGE67909T20100810.
- ↑ Migdon, Brooke (December 14, 2021). "US LGBTQ+ population hits 20 million". https://thehill.com/changing-america/respect/diversity-inclusion/585711-us-lgbtq-population-hits-20-million.
- ↑ Multiple sources:
- Negotiating the Good Life. Taylor & Francis. 5 July 2017. ISBN 978-1-351-91544-1. https://books.google.com/books?id=BkUrDwAAQBAJ&dq=%2230+million%22+gay+usa&pg=PA156. Retrieved 2022-01-28.
- Ideas and Movements that Shaped America. ABC-CLIO. 28 July 2015. ISBN 978-1-61069-252-6. https://books.google.com/books?id=bZMeCgAAQBAJ&dq=%2230+million%22+gay+usa&pg=PA463. Retrieved 2022-01-28.
- Berman, Larry; Murphy, Bruce Allen (2001). Approaching Democracy. Prentice Hall. ISBN 978-0-13-087111-4. https://books.google.com/books?id=4zLhjj-XcDMC&q=%2230%20million%20homosexual%22. Retrieved 2022-01-28.
- British Journal of Venereal Diseases. British Medical Association. 1981. https://books.google.com/books?id=SAB4fkpSc2UC&q=%2230%20million%22%20. Retrieved 2022-01-28.
- "New Generation Digital Banks". https://www.nbcnews.com/nbc-out/out-news/new-generation-digital-banks-bets-minority-markets-rcna924.
- ↑ "Estimated LGBT Purchasing Power and LGBT Population". August 19, 2020. http://www.lgbt-capital.com/docs/Estimated_LGBT-GDP_(table)_-_2020.pdf.
- ↑ "BRAZIL | Out Leadership". April 19, 2018. https://outleadership.com/countries/brazil/,%20https://outleadership.com/countries/brazil/.[yes|permanent dead link|dead link}}]
- ↑ "What Can LGBT+ People Expect from the Bolsonaro Government?". November 7, 2018. https://www.huffingtonpost.co.uk/entry/lgbt-bolsonaro-government_uk_5bdbaacfe4b042b1178e7c02.
- ↑ "NIGERIA | Out Leadership". April 25, 2018. https://outleadership.com/countries/nigeria/,%20https://outleadership.com/countries/nigeria/.[yes|permanent dead link|dead link}}]
- ↑ "Estimated LGBT Purchasing Power and LGBT Population". August 19, 2020. http://www.lgbt-capital.com/docs/Estimated_LGBT-GDP_(table)_-_2020.pdf.
- ↑ "Estimated LGBT Purchasing Power and LGBT Population". August 19, 2020. http://www.lgbt-capital.com/docs/Estimated_LGBT-GDP_(table)_-_2020.pdf.
- ↑ "Estimated LGBT Purchasing Power and LGBT Population". August 19, 2020. http://www.lgbt-capital.com/docs/Estimated_LGBT-GDP_(table)_-_2020.pdf.
- ↑ "Estimated LGBT Purchasing Power and LGBT Population". August 19, 2020. http://www.lgbt-capital.com/docs/Estimated_LGBT-GDP_(table)_-_2020.pdf.
- ↑ "Estimated LGBT Purchasing Power and LGBT Population". August 19, 2020. http://www.lgbt-capital.com/docs/Estimated_LGBT-GDP_(table)_-_2020.pdf.
- ↑ "Estimated LGBT Purchasing Power and LGBT Population". August 19, 2020. http://www.lgbt-capital.com/docs/Estimated_LGBT-GDP_(table)_-_2020.pdf.
- ↑ "FRANCE | Out Leadership". April 19, 2018. https://outleadership.com/countries/brazil/,%20https://outleadership.com/countries/brazil/.[yes|permanent dead link|dead link}}]
- ↑ "Estimated LGBT Purchasing Power and LGBT Population". August 19, 2020. http://www.lgbt-capital.com/docs/Estimated_LGBT-GDP_(table)_-_2020.pdf.
- ↑ "Estimated LGBT Purchasing Power and LGBT Population". August 19, 2020. http://www.lgbt-capital.com/docs/Estimated_LGBT-GDP_(table)_-_2020.pdf.
- ↑ "UNITED KINGDOM | Out Leadership". April 19, 2018. https://outleadership.com/countries/brazil/,%20https://outleadership.com/countries/brazil/.[yes|permanent dead link|dead link}}]
- ↑ "Estimated LGBT Purchasing Power and LGBT Population". August 19, 2020. http://www.lgbt-capital.com/docs/Estimated_LGBT-GDP_(table)_-_2020.pdf.
- ↑ "Estimated LGBT Purchasing Power and LGBT Population". August 19, 2020. http://www.lgbt-capital.com/docs/Estimated_LGBT-GDP_(table)_-_2020.pdf.
- ↑ "Estimated LGBT Purchasing Power and LGBT Population". August 19, 2020. http://www.lgbt-capital.com/docs/Estimated_LGBT-GDP_(table)_-_2020.pdf.
- ↑ "Estimated LGBT Purchasing Power and LGBT Population". August 19, 2020. http://www.lgbt-capital.com/docs/Estimated_LGBT-GDP_(table)_-_2020.pdf.
- ↑ "Estimated LGBT Purchasing Power and LGBT Population". August 19, 2020. http://www.lgbt-capital.com/docs/Estimated_LGBT-GDP_(table)_-_2020.pdf.
- ↑ "Estimated LGBT Purchasing Power and LGBT Population". August 19, 2020. http://www.lgbt-capital.com/docs/Estimated_LGBT-GDP_(table)_-_2020.pdf.
- ↑ "Estimated LGBT Purchasing Power and LGBT Population". August 19, 2020. http://www.lgbt-capital.com/docs/Estimated_LGBT-GDP_(table)_-_2020.pdf.
- ↑ "Estimated LGBT Purchasing Power and LGBT Population". August 19, 2020. http://www.lgbt-capital.com/docs/Estimated_LGBT-GDP_(table)_-_2020.pdf.
Further reading
- "Doubt cast on 'gay gene'". BBC News. 23 April 1999. http://news.bbc.co.uk/hi/english/sci/tech/newsid_325000/325979.stm.
- "The biological evidence challenged". Scientific American 270 (5): 50–5. May 1994. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0594-50. PMID 8197445. Bibcode: 1994SciAm.270e..50B. https://www.pbs.org/wgbh/pages/frontline/shows/assault/genetics/biochallenge.html.
- "Homosexual orientation in males: evolutionary and ethological aspects". Neuro Endocrinology Letters 22 (6): 393–400. December 2001. PMID 11781535. http://evolution.anthro.univie.ac.at/institutes/urbanethology/resources/pdf/NEL220601R01_Muscarella_.pdf.
- "The neurodevelopment of human sexual orientation". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 29 (7): 1057–66. 2005. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.03.002. PMID 16143171.
- "Fetal effects on sexual behavior and aggression in young and old female mice treated with estrogen and testosterone". Hormones and Behavior 18 (2): 117–29. June 1984. doi:10.1016/0018-506X(84)90037-0. PMID 6539747.
- "Biological Research on Women's Sexual Orientations: Evaluating the Scientific Evidence". Journal of Social Issues 56 (2): 267–282. 2000. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00165.
- "Intrauterine position effects". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 26 (6): 665–78. October 2002. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00038-6. PMID 12479841.
- "Evidence for a biological influence in male homosexuality". Scientific American 270 (5): 44–9. May 1994. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0594-44. PMID 8197444. Bibcode: 1994SciAm.270e..44L.
- "Sexual characteristics of adult female mice are correlated with their blood testosterone levels during prenatal development". Science 208 (4444): 597–9. May 1980. doi:10.1126/science.7367881. PMID 7367881. Bibcode: 1980Sci...208..597V.
Original source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Biology and sexual orientation.
Read more |